'Patient R' makes scientists revise theory of how brain works

There's so much we don't know about the brain.
We use techniques like functional neuroimaging to find out which brain regions light up while a subject performs an activity, but whether or not that really means that area is responsible for that function can remain a mystery.
And that's why Patient R, a man who had damage in the three areas of the brain, has proved so useful to researchers: His case has disproved that the three areas of the brain previously thought critical for self-awareness actually are responsible for it.
Even more importantly, his case could upend traditional notions that specific brain regions have specific duties. The brain might actually be more flexible, with certain areas taking on new functions depending on other events in the brain.
Patient R
Self-awareness is a pretty high-level cognitive function.
It's a step (or several steps?) beyond consciousness (as described by Scientific American), because consciousness is just having thoughts, while self-awareness is recognizing that you have thoughts, plus then thinking about your thoughts. Self-aware beings also know their traits, feelings and behaviors. (That's why scientists have asserted that a number of animals are conscious, but none besides humans are said to have self-awareness.)
Neuroimaging studies suggested that self-awareness comes from three areas of the brain: the insular cortex, the anterior cingulate cortex, and the medial prefrontal cortex.
So when David Rudrauf at the University of Iowa in Iowa City heard that Patient R, also known as Roger, suffered extensive brain damage in exactly these areas, he thought Patient R would be a good way to corroborate the neuroimaging studies on self-awareness. Only ten percent of Roger's insula remained, as did only one percent of the anterior cingulate cortex.
And yet, in the experiments, Roger still exhibited a sense of self that allowed him to recognize himself in the mirror and in photographs. He also passed the "tickle test," which is based on the idea that you can't tickle yourself. He could always distinguish between when he was being tickled by others or by himself, and was always more "tickled" by others.
He could even make jokes that showed he had a sense of self. When a researcher asked Roger for permission to tickle, he, jokingly referring to his sweaty armpits, responded, "Got a towel?"
Significance
The authors, who published their study in PLOS One, concluded that the three areas of the brain previously considered critical for self-awareness aren't.
Additionally, they suggest that self-awareness is a diffuse cognitive process that uses many parts of the brain, including those not located in the cerebral cortex.
Rudrauf also told New Scientist that self-awareness and other high-level cognitive functions "involve layers of abstraction and mechanisms that cannot be explained by standard functional-neuroanatomy. ... We would all like simple answers to complicated questions, and we tend to oversimplify our conceptions about the brain and the mind."
Related on SmartPlanet:
- Memory loss can begin as early as 45, study says
- A pain-erasing pill is no longer the stuff of movies
- Video: Subjects move virtual helicopter with thoughts
- Detecting Alzheimer's in the way you walk
- Why we like to brag and overshare
- How your cat could be making you 'crazy'
- Scanner finds some brain-dead patients aware
- Mind-reading tech reconstructs moving images others see
via: ScienceDaily, Scientific American, New Scientist
This post was originally published on Smartplanet.com